Abstract

The purpose of this study was to take a new approach in showing how the central nervous system might encode time at the supra-second level using recurrent neural nets (RNNs). This approach utilizes units with a delayed feedback, whose feedback weight determines the temporal properties of specific neurons in the network architecture. When these feedback neurons are coupled, they form a multilayered dynamical system that can be used to model temporal responses to steps of input in multidimensional systems. The timing network was implemented using separate recurrent “Go” and “No-Go” neural processing units to process an individual stimulus indicating the time of reward availability. Outputs from these distinct units on each time step are converted to a pulse reflecting a weighted sum of the separate Go and No-Go signals. This output pulse then drives an integrator unit, whose feedback weight and input weights shape the pulse distribution. This system was used to model empirical data from rodents performing in an instrumental “peak interval timing” task for two stimuli, Tone and Flash. For each of these stimuli, reward availability was signaled after different times from stimulus onset during training. Rodent performance was assessed on non-rewarded trials, following training, with each stimulus tested individually and simultaneously in a stimulus compound. The associated weights in the Go/No-Go network were trained using experimental data showing the mean distribution of bar press rates across an 80 s period in which a tone stimulus signaled reward after 5 s and a flash stimulus after 30 s from stimulus onset. Different Go/No-Go systems were used for each stimulus, but the weighted output of each fed into a final recurrent integrator unit, whose weights were unmodifiable. The recurrent neural net (RNN) model was implemented using Matlab and Matlab’s machine learning tools were utilized to train the network using the data from non-rewarded trials. The neural net output accurately fit the temporal distribution of tone and flash-initiated bar press data. Furthermore, a “Temporal Averaging” effect was also obtained when the flash and tone stimuli were combined. These results indicated that the system combining tone and flash responses were not superposed as in a linear system, but that there was a non-linearity, which interacted between tone and flash. In order to achieve an accurate fit to the empirical averaging data it was necessary to implement non-linear “saliency functions” that limited the output signal of each stimulus to the final integrator when the other was co-present. The model suggests that the central nervous system encodes timing generation as a dynamical system whose timing properties are embedded in the connection weights of the system. In this way, event timing is coded similar to the way other sensory-motor systems, such as the vestibulo-ocular and optokinetic systems, which combine sensory inputs from the vestibular and visual systems to generate the temporal aspects of compensatory eye movements.

Highlights

  • The temporal encoding of significant events in neural structures has become an important consideration in a wide range of adaptive behaviors

  • We demonstrated that the model could be trained to produce the different response functions we obtained with rats performing in a peak timing task when one stimulus (Tone) indicated reward availability after 5 s and a second stimulus (Flash) after 30 s

  • Both the rats and the model responded to the stimulus compound by “averaging” the two independent temporal estimates to reward such that maximal responding occurred at an interval intermediate between the two signaled by the individual stimuli (Swanton et al, 2009; Swanton and Matell, 2011; Matell and Henning, 2013; Matell and Kurti, 2014; Delamater and Nicolas, 2015; De Corte and Matell, 2016a,b)

Read more

Summary

Introduction

The temporal encoding of significant events in neural structures has become an important consideration in a wide range of adaptive behaviors. Other studies on interval timing showed that a visual stimulus associated with a standard duration could be identified amongst other intervals that were given to subjects (Wearden et al, 1997). This work showed that this estimation of time intervals could be scaled for standard deviation as the interval was increased according to Weber’s law (Wearden et al, 1997). Following Pavlov’s work on conditioning (Pavlov, 1927) and subsequent work on operant conditioning (Skinner, 1938, 1951), there has been growing interest in studying how animals learn to time the arrival of key events, such as reward (Staddon and Higa, 1999; Staddon, 2005). At issue is how the animal encodes the time at which food becomes available and distributes its behavior

Objectives
Results
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.