Abstract

The mechanism underlying visual-induced auditory interaction is still under discussion. Here, we provide evidence that the mirror mechanism underlies visual–auditory interactions. In this study, visual stimuli were divided into two major groups—mirror stimuli that were able to activate mirror neurons and non-mirror stimuli that were not able to activate mirror neurons. The two groups were further divided into six subgroups as follows: visual speech-related mirror stimuli, visual speech-irrelevant mirror stimuli, and non-mirror stimuli with four different luminance levels. Participants were 25 children with cochlear implants (CIs) who underwent an event-related potential (ERP) and speech recognition task. The main results were as follows: (1) there were significant differences in P1, N1, and P2 ERPs between mirror stimuli and non-mirror stimuli; (2) these ERP differences between mirror and non-mirror stimuli were partly driven by Brodmann areas 41 and 42 in the superior temporal gyrus; (3) ERP component differences between visual speech-related mirror and non-mirror stimuli were partly driven by Brodmann area 39 (visual speech area), which was not observed when comparing the visual speech-irrelevant stimulus and non-mirror groups; and (4) ERPs evoked by visual speech-related mirror stimuli had more components correlated with speech recognition than ERPs evoked by non-mirror stimuli, while ERPs evoked by speech-irrelevant mirror stimuli were not significantly different to those induced by the non-mirror stimuli. These results indicate the following: (1) mirror and non-mirror stimuli differ in their associated neural activation; (2) the visual–auditory interaction possibly led to ERP differences, as Brodmann areas 41 and 42 constitute the primary auditory cortex; (3) mirror neurons could be responsible for the ERP differences, considering that Brodmann area 39 is associated with processing information about speech-related mirror stimuli; and (4) ERPs evoked by visual speech-related mirror stimuli could better reflect speech recognition ability. These results support the hypothesis that a mirror mechanism underlies visual–auditory interactions.

Highlights

  • Visual–auditory interactions represent the interference between the visual system and the auditory system (Bulkin and Groh, 2006)

  • Significant differences were observed in the P1, N1, and P2 components between event-related potential (ERP) evoked by mirror stimuli and those evoked by non-mirror stimuli (Figure 1C)

  • On comparing the ERPs elicited by the mirror stimuli and non-mirror stimuli, the results indicated that the mirror neurons were more strongly activated in the “motor action” condition than in the functional cortex

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Visual–auditory interactions represent the interference between the visual system and the auditory system (Bulkin and Groh, 2006). A classic example of a visual–auditory interaction is the McGurk effect, in which video lip movements for [ga], dubbed by syllable sound [ba], lead to the auditory illusion of the fused syllable [da] (McGurk and MacDonald, 1976) This indicates that the visual system interferes with the auditory system. Neuroimaging evidence has suggested that cross-modal plasticity might underlie visual–auditory interactions in patients with hearing loss (Finney et al, 2001; Mao and Pallas, 2013; Stropahl and Debener, 2017) This could indicate that a loss of auditory input causes the visual cortex to “take up” the auditory cortex via cross-modal plasticity and causes an interference of the visual system on the auditory system

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.