Abstract

Moths have evolved auditory channels under predation pressure from insectivorous bats that emit ultrasonic pulses for capturing prey, including moths. Tympanate moths perform evasive behavior in response to echolocation calls of bats, but they also utilize ultrasonic signals mostly generated by males close to an intended female mate in the context of courtship. Unlike calling songs used to advertise the presence and sexual attractiveness of the signaler, courtship songs need not always be acoustically conspicuous. Male courtship songs are predominantly soft but sufficient for detection by a nearby potential mate. Quiet courtship songs are thought to effectively avoid being eavesdropped by gleaning bats, acoustic parasitoids, and conspecific competitors, i.e., rival males. However, males of some moth species generate loud courtship songs. In the present study, the duration of courtship song, in addition to the sound level of the song was predicted to affect the likelihood of being perceived by eavesdroppers. Loud and lengthy courtship songs, which are easily exploited by eavesdroppers, would be expected to rarely evolve, because a female receiver close to a male emitting a conspicuous song would also be exposed to strong predation pressure. This study explored the relationship between the peak sound level and the duration of single song bouts in 26 moth species from the following families: Noctuidae, Erebidae, Crambidae, Pyralidae, and Geometridae. The softest and loudest songs with mean peak sound levels of 64 and 129dB peSPL had mean durations of 1900 and 312 ms, respectively, whereas the shortest and longest songs with mean durations of 110 and 8839 ms had mean peak sound levels of 102 and 74 dB peSPL, respectively. Thus, the peak sound level and duration of courtship song exhibited a significant negative relationship. Although the energetic cost of producing song and the size of the sound-producing organ might also affect the relationship, the data support the conclusion that acoustic moths have adaptively evolved ultrasonic courtship songs with properties between “soft-and-long” and “loud-and-short” to avoid eavesdroppers.

Highlights

  • Animals have evolved communication signals for mating

  • Moths of all growth stages were maintained under a 16 h light:8 h dark photo-regime in experimental rooms at 20 ± 1◦C for three Canadian species and 24 ± 1◦C for nine Japanese species [three noctuids (Spodoptera picta, Spodoptera exigua, and Spodoptera pecten), one erebid (Lithosia quadra), three crambids (Ostrinia zealis, Ostrinia palustralis, and Ostrinia latipennis), and two pyralids (Paralipsa gularis and Endotricha icelusalis)]

  • We have shown that a negative relationship between loudness and duration exists in the ultrasonic courtship songs of male moths

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Animals have evolved communication signals for mating. During a mating sequence involving emission of a signal to a focal receiver i.e., usually a female, an unintended receiver has a chance to exploit the signal to find, locate, and hunt or parasitize a conspicuous signaler (Zuk and Kolluru, 1998). Loud calling songs are Inconspicuous Courtship Song in Moth widespread in acoustic animals because of their usefulness to advertise the presence of the caller and attract mating partners from a long distance, but conspicuous songs simultaneously convey information on the location of the caller to predators, including bats, birds, and reptiles (Tuttle and Ryan, 1981; Sakaluk and Belwood, 1984; Bell, 1985; Tuttle et al, 1985; Bailey and Haythornthwaite, 1998; Igaune et al, 2008; Jones et al, 2011); parasitoids and blood-sucking flies (Cade, 1975; Walker, 1993; Bernal et al, 2006; Bernal and de Silva, 2015); and rival males of the same species (Cade, 1980; Bailey and Field, 2000; Zuk et al, 2006; Bailey et al, 2010). As countertactics to avoid predation by eavesdropping bats, singing males of Achroia grisella, like crickets and katydids (Spangler, 1984; Nolen and Hoy, 1986; Libersat and Hoy, 1991; Faure and Hoy, 2000; Schulze and Schul, 2001), cease emission of their calling songs, and the females stop orientation toward calling males, when they detect ultrasonic echolocation calls of hunting bats (Spangler, 1984; Greenfield and Baker, 2003; Greig and Greenfield, 2004; Rodríguez and Greenfield, 2004; Cordes et al, 2014)

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call