Abstract

Kinesthesia is our sense of limb motion, and allows us to gauge the speed, direction, and amplitude of our movements. Over half of stroke survivors have significant impairments in kinesthesia, which leads to greatly reduced recovery and function in everyday activities. Despite the high reported incidence of kinesthetic deficits after stroke, very little is known about how damage beyond just primary somatosensory areas affects kinesthesia. Stroke provides an ideal model to examine structure-function relationships specific to kinesthetic processing, by comparing lesion location with behavioral impairment. To examine this relationship, we performed voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping and statistical region of interest analyses on a large sample of sub-acute stroke subjects (N = 142) and compared kinesthetic performance with stroke lesion location. Subjects with first unilateral, ischemic stroke underwent neuroimaging and a comprehensive robotic kinesthetic assessment (~9 days post-stroke). The robotic exoskeleton measured subjects' ability to perform a kinesthetic mirror-matching task of the upper limbs without vision. The robot moved the stroke-affected arm and subjects' mirror-matched the movement with the unaffected arm. We found that lesions both within and outside primary somatosensory cortex were associated with significant kinesthetic impairments. Further, sub-components of kinesthesia were associated with different lesion locations. Impairments in speed perception were primarily associated with lesions to the right post-central and supramarginal gyri whereas impairments in amplitude of movement perception were primarily associated with lesions in the right pre-central gyrus, anterior insula, and superior temporal gyrus. Impairments in perception of movement direction were associated with lesions to bilateral post-central and supramarginal gyri, right superior temporal gyrus and parietal operculum. All measures of impairment shared a common association with damage to the right supramarginal gyrus. These results suggest that processing of kinesthetic information occurs beyond traditional sensorimotor areas. Additionally, this dissociation between kinesthetic sub-components may indicate specialized processing in these brain areas that form a larger distributed network.

Highlights

  • Proprioception is traditionally thought to be comprised of two components, position sense and movement sense (Sherrington, 1907; Goodwin et al, 1972)

  • We examined a large sample of stroke survivors with cerebral lesions using voxel-based lesion symptom mapping (VLSM) (Bates et al, 2003), and a statistical region of interest analysis

  • Subjects were excluded for the following reasons: clinical diagnosis of stroke prior to the current one, hemorrhagic stroke, stroke affecting both sides of the brain, stroke in the brainstem, no identifiable lesion on MRI or CT, pre-existing neurological disorder, orthopedic problems, neuropathy, or pain in either upper extremity, or inability to follow the instructions for the robotic assessment due to aphasia, language barriers, apraxia, or cognitive deficits

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Proprioception is traditionally thought to be comprised of two components, position sense and movement sense (kinesthesia) (Sherrington, 1907; Goodwin et al, 1972). Humans have both static (position sensitive) and dynamic (movement sensitive) peripheral muscle receptors (Proske and Gandevia, 2012). Kinesthetic impairments are associated with reduced functional independence (Torre et al, 2013). Case series level evidence has demonstrated post-stroke brain lesion locations associated with “abnormal” position sense (including the thalamus, internal capsule, and post-central gyrus) (Kim, 1992, 2007; Tong et al, 2010). Much less is known about the underlying neuroanatomy of impaired kinesthesia (Kenzie et al, 2014)

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.