Abstract

Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptoms include behavioral avoidance which is acquired and tends to increase with time. This avoidance may represent a general learning bias; indeed, individuals with PTSD are often faster than controls on acquiring conditioned responses based on physiologically-aversive feedback. However, it is not clear whether this learning bias extends to cognitive feedback, or to learning from both reward and punishment. Here, male veterans with self-reported current, severe PTSD symptoms (PTSS group) or with few or no PTSD symptoms (control group) completed a probabilistic classification task that included both reward-based and punishment-based trials, where feedback could take the form of reward, punishment, or an ambiguous “no-feedback” outcome that could signal either successful avoidance of punishment or failure to obtain reward. The PTSS group outperformed the control group in total points obtained; the PTSS group specifically performed better than the control group on reward-based trials, with no difference on punishment-based trials. To better understand possible mechanisms underlying observed performance, we used a reinforcement learning model of the task, and applied maximum likelihood estimation techniques to derive estimated parameters describing individual participants’ behavior. Estimations of the reinforcement value of the no-feedback outcome were significantly greater in the control group than the PTSS group, suggesting that the control group was more likely to value this outcome as positively reinforcing (i.e., signaling successful avoidance of punishment). This is consistent with the control group’s generally poorer performance on reward trials, where reward feedback was to be obtained in preference to the no-feedback outcome. Differences in the interpretation of ambiguous feedback may contribute to the facilitated reinforcement learning often observed in PTSD patients, and may in turn provide new insight into how pathological behaviors are acquired and maintained in PTSD.

Highlights

  • In the wake of exposure to a traumatic event, some individuals develop post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which includes reexperiencing, avoidance, emotional numbing, and hyperarousal symptoms

  • There was no difference in Adult Measure of Behavioural Inhibition (AMBI) or Retrospective Measure of Behavioural Inhibition (RMBI) scores among veterans with vs. without a history of exposure to combat but AMBI scores were significantly higher in the PTSD symptoms (PTSS) than control group (t(85) = 5.60, p,0.001), RMBI scores did not differ between groups (t(85) = 1.52, p = 0.131)

  • Because a fairly large number of participants maintained nearchance performance on both the reward and punishment tasks, we considered the subset of participants who achieved at least 65% optimal responding on the reward or punishment trials

Read more

Summary

Introduction

In the wake of exposure to a traumatic event, some individuals develop post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), which includes reexperiencing, avoidance, emotional numbing, and hyperarousal symptoms. A large percentage of trauma victims who do not meet full symptom criteria for PTSD diagnosis present with a number of PTSD symptoms; such subclinical or subthreshold PTSD may cause significant distress and functional impairment [3,4,5,6]. In both clinical and nonclinical groups, arousal symptoms may appear earlier than, and predict the emergence of, other symptom categories [7], while avoidance and re-experiencing symptoms may follow a more linear trajectory [8]. An influential model of PTSD assumes that some PTSD symptoms reflect classicallyconditioned associations; initially-neutral cues (conditioned stimuli, CS) that are present at the time of exposure to a traumatic event (unconditioned stimuli, US) become associated with the strong emotional responses (unconditioned response, UR)

Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.