Abstract

High trait positive affect (PA) protects against depressive symptoms through cognitive responses such as rumination. However, how rumination in response to positive emotions (positive rumination) protects against depressive symptoms while rumination in response to negative emotions (brooding) predicts depressive symptoms is poorly understood. We hypothesized that (a) positive rumination and brooding represent a shared cognitive process of affect amplification on distinct affective content and (b) less brooding and greater positive rumination would distinctly mediate greater trait PA in predicting fewer depressive symptoms. Our prospective design among 321 adults first compared three confirmatory factor analysis models of the relationship between brooding and positive rumination. We then utilized structural equation modeling to examine whether brooding and positive rumination mediated the relationship between trait PA and depressive symptoms, controlling for baseline depressive symptoms, trait negative affect (NA), and the distinct effects of each mediator. Results supported a conceptualization of brooding and positive rumination as distinct but related constructs, represented as a common process of affect amplification to explain how rumination may amplify resilience or risk in predicting depressive symptoms (χ = 195.07, Δχ = 8.78, p < .001, CFI = .91, RMSEA = .07). Furthermore, positive rumination and brooding were distinctly predicted by trait PA, suggesting that trait PA exerts distinct effects on protective and risk forms of rumination. Less brooding mediated the relationship between greater trait PA and fewer depressive symptoms (β = -.04, p = .012), but positive rumination did not (β = .02, p = .517). Rumination may represent a protective and a risk factor, which may better enable individuals who brood to redirect their rumination on positive content and thereby reduce their risk of depressive symptoms.

Highlights

  • High trait positive affect (PA) protects against depressive symptoms through cognitive responses such as rumination

  • Part 1 conducted a Confirmatory Factor Analysis (CFA) of Week 1 measures of brooding and positive rumination and sequentially tested three nested models (Figure 1): positive rumination and brooding modeled as distinct constructs (Model 1), distinct but related constructs (Model 2), and the same construct (Model 3)

  • We proposed that the distinct variances of positive rumination and brooding would represent positive affective content and negative affective content, respectively, which is consistent with how literature describes them as distinct forms of rumination

Read more

Summary

Participants and Procedure

Depressive symptoms were measured at Week 1 as a covariate and seven weeks later as the dependent variable with the 20-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977). Participants rated how they felt and behaved over the past week. Responses ranged from 1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always) for items such as, “When you are feeling happy, how often do you think about how happy you feel?” and “When you are feeling happy, how often do you think ‘I am living up to my potential’?” Higher scores indicated greater positive rumination.

Results
Part 1: CFA
Part 2: Mediation Analyses
Discussion
Limitations and Future
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call