Abstract

To what extent is handwritten word production based on phonological codes? A few studies conducted in Western languages have recently provided evidence showing that phonology contributes to the retrieval of graphemic properties in written output tasks. Less is known about how orthographic production works in languages with non-alphabetic scripts such as written Chinese. We report a Stroop study in which Chinese participants wrote the color of characters on a digital graphic tablet; characters were either neutral, or homophonic to the target (congruent), or homophonic to an alternative (incongruent). Facilitation was found from congruent homophonic distractors, but only when the homophone shared the same tone with the target. This finding suggests a contribution of phonology to written word production. A second experiment served as a control experiment to exclude the possibility that the effect in Experiment 1 had an exclusively semantic locus. Overall, the findings offer new insight into the relative contribution of phonology to handwriting, particularly in non-Western languages.

Highlights

  • How is the spelling of words mentally represented? Over the last few decades, this issue has attracted substantial attention in psycholinguistic research

  • The results showed that writing latencies on color naming were slower for incongruent trials relative to neutral control trials at all three stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA), and this was the case irrespective of whether or not characters and color names shared the tone

  • This interference effect likely reflects the fact that in the incongruent conditions, Chinese writers activated the phonological information of the character and co-activated an incongruent color name via shared phonology, creating a conflict with the selection of the to-be-written color response

Read more

Summary

Introduction

How is the spelling of words mentally represented? Over the last few decades, this issue has attracted substantial attention in psycholinguistic research. In other words, when an individual produces orthographic output, she first translates meaning into inner speech, and this phonological code is transformed into orthographic representations This view is in line with the observation that spoken language precedes written production ontogenetically and phylogenetically (e.g., Scinto, 1986). It fits most individuals’ introspection about how writing is achieved (Hotopf, 1980), and it accounts for spelling and typing errors such as homophone substitutions (e.g., there spelled as “their”) and production of phonologically plausible non-words (e.g., dearth spelled as “dirth”; Aitchison and Todd, 1982)

Objectives
Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.