Abstract
Many tasks require the skilled interaction of both hands, such as eating with knife and fork or keyboard typing. However, our understanding of the behavioural and neurophysiological mechanisms underpinning bimanual motor learning is still sparse. Here, we aimed to address this by first characterising learning-related changes of different levels of bimanual interaction and second investigating how beta tACS modulates these learning-related changes. To explore early bimanual motor learning, we designed a novel bimanual motor learning task. In the task, a force grip device held in each hand (controlling x- and y-axis separately) was used to move a cursor along a path of streets at different angles (0°, 22.5°, 45°, 67.5°, and 90°). Each street corresponded to specific force ratios between hands, which resulted in different levels of hand interaction, i.e., unimanual (Uni, i.e., 0°, 90°), bimanual with equal force (Bieq, 45°), and bimanual with unequal force (Biuneq 22.5°, 67.5°). In experiment 1, 40 healthy participants performed the task for 45 min with a minimum of 100 trials. We found that the novel task induced improvements in movement time and error, with no trade-off between movement time and error, and with distinct patterns for the three levels of bimanual interaction. In experiment 2, we performed a between-subjects, double-blind study in 54 healthy participants to explore the effect of phase synchrony between both sensorimotor cortices using tACS at the individual’s beta peak frequency. The individual’s beta peak frequency was quantified using electroencephalography. 20 min of 2 mA peak-to-peak amplitude tACS was applied during task performance (40 min). Participants either received in-phase (0° phase shift), out-of-phase (90° phase shift), or sham (3 s of stimulation) tACS. We replicated the behavioural results of experiment 1, however, beta tACS did not modulate motor learning. Overall, the novel bimanual motor task allows to characterise bimanual motor learning with different levels of bimanual interaction. This should pave the way for future neuroimaging studies to further investigate the underlying mechanism of bimanual motor learning.
Highlights
In everyday life, we perform countless movements with our hands
Bimanual interactions are mainly studied using simple finger tapping, sequence tapping and the simultaneous or alternating flexion/extension of individual fingers (Shammi et al, 1998; Bangert et al, 2010; Takeuchi et al, 2012; Sallard et al, 2014; Koppelmans et al, 2015; Loehrer et al, 2016; Kajal et al, 2017). These studies have advanced our understanding of which regions are involved in bimanual interaction and how they communicate with each other, but the largely artificial tasks used remain distant to daily life and often do not require learning
In experiment 2, we investigated test the role of hemispheric synchrony of beta activity while learning the task with inphase and out-ofphase transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) at the individual beta-peak frequency above both sensorimotor cortices
Summary
We perform countless movements with our hands. Some, such as writing, drawing, or eating with chopsticks, require one hand alone, whereas others require the skilled interaction of both hands. Bimanual interactions are mainly studied using simple finger tapping, sequence tapping and the simultaneous or alternating flexion/extension of individual fingers (Shammi et al, 1998; Bangert et al, 2010; Takeuchi et al, 2012; Sallard et al, 2014; Koppelmans et al, 2015; Loehrer et al, 2016; Kajal et al, 2017) These studies have advanced our understanding of which regions are involved in bimanual interaction and how they communicate with each other (see for review Swinnen, 2002; Swinnen and Gooijers, 2015), but the largely artificial tasks used remain distant to daily life and often do not require learning
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.