Abstract

Generally defined, chunking is a process through which one reorganizes or groups presented information to compress information; it is one of the best-known methods of increasing the amount of information stored in memory. Chunking can occur by two different means: either through strategic reorganization based on familiarity or prior knowledge, or through grouping based on perceptual characteristics. An example of the former is using knowledge of acronyms to break a string of letters (e.g., AWOLNASAMIA) into smaller, separate groups (i.e., AWOL, NASA, MIA). In the case of the latter, more common with visual stimuli, one can form groups on the basis of similarity or proximity. Although both methods are considered part of the general phenomenon of chunking, it is the goal-directed, strategic chunking that is the focus of this piece. Although the process of chunking has been discussed as a mnemonic strategy in William James's Principles of Psychology (1890), it is most widely known through George Miller's paper, “The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two.” Miller (1956) primarily reviewed several studies that examined capacity limits in immediate recall; across various types of stimuli, a consistent recall limit of between five and nine items was observed. As a secondary emphasis, Miller also discussed recoding and subsequent implications on estimates of immediate memory capacity. Miller observed that if information was recoded into meaningful units (called chunks), this increased the amount of information that could be recalled, and thereby increased immediate memory span. This occurs because increased meaning through chunking or recoding increases the size of each respective chunk (e.g., Tulving and Patkau, 1962; Chase and Simon, 1973; Simon, 1974), but the number of chunks that can be stored in short-term memory remains constant, typically limited to four or fewer items (e.g., Cowan, 2001; Gobet and Clarkson, 2004; Mathy and Feldman, 2012). Despite the fact that Miller published his paper nearly 60 years prior, our understanding of chunking remains incomplete. In particular, though many chunking papers use a variety of methods to measure how chunks are formed and retrieved, it is unclear whether the majority of these methods of measuring chunks accurately reflect the internal cognitive processes that are involved in chunk formation. Before discussing this problem in further detail, I will briefly review well-known methods of measuring chunks and how these methods have been used in previous research (see Gilchrist and Cowan, 2012, for a detailed discussion of chunking and these measurement methods). For present purposes, I will be restricting these measurement methods to those involved in deliberate and goal-directed chunking of verbal materials, as these typically require more effortful processing.

Highlights

  • Defined, chunking is a process through which one reorganizes or groups presented information to compress information; it is one of the best-known methods of increasing the amount of information stored in memory

  • This occurs because increased meaning through chunking or recoding increases the size of each respective chunk (e.g., Tulving and Patkau, 1962; Chase and Simon, 1973; Simon, 1974), but the number of chunks that can be stored in short-term memory remains constant, typically limited to four or fewer items (e.g., Cowan, 2001; Gobet and Clarkson, 2004; Mathy and Feldman, 2012)

  • Though many chunking papers use a variety of methods to measure how chunks are formed and retrieved, it is unclear whether the majority of these methods of measuring chunks accurately reflect the internal cognitive processes that are involved in chunk formation

Read more

Summary

Frontiers in Psychology

Miller observed that if information was recoded into meaningful units (called chunks), this increased the amount of information that could be recalled, and thereby increased immediate memory span This occurs because increased meaning through chunking or recoding increases the size of each respective chunk (e.g., Tulving and Patkau, 1962; Chase and Simon, 1973; Simon, 1974), but the number of chunks that can be stored in short-term memory remains constant, typically limited to four or fewer items (e.g., Cowan, 2001; Gobet and Clarkson, 2004; Mathy and Feldman, 2012).

How should we measure chunks?
The Problem with Measurement Methods and the Way Forward
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call