Abstract

How do people decide how general a causal relationship is, in terms of the entities or situations it applies to? What features do people use to decide whether a new situation is governed by a new causal law or an old one? How can people make these difficult judgments in a fast, efficient way? We address these questions in two experiments that ask participants to generalize from one (Experiment 1) or several (Experiment 2) causal interactions between pairs of objects. In each case, participants see an agent object act on a recipient object, causing some changes to the recipient. In line with the human capacity for few-shot concept learning, we find systematic patterns of causal generalizations favoring simpler causal laws that extend over categories of similar objects. In Experiment 1, we find that participants’ inferences are shaped by the order of the generalization questions they are asked. In both experiments, we find an asymmetry in the formation of causal categories: participants preferentially identify causal laws with features of the agent objects rather than recipients. To explain this, we develop a computational model that combines program induction (about the hidden causal laws) with non-parametric category inference (about their domains of influence). We demonstrate that our modeling approach can both explain the order effect in Experiment 1 and the causal asymmetry, and outperforms a naïve Bayesian account while providing a computationally plausible mechanism for real-world causal generalization.

Highlights

  • People readily generalize from familiar causal relationships to novel ones, using the features of prospective objects as a guide

  • We find that our local laws and our new process model better explain our behavioral data than a purely normative account, including explaining a novel generalization-order effect observed in Experiment 1

  • As with Experiment 1, we compared participants’ generalizations to a random Baseline model, a Universal Causal Laws (UnCaLa) model and a Local Causal Laws (LoCaLa) model, again using maximum likelihood and BIC to account for different numbers of parameters

Read more

Summary

Introduction

People readily generalize from familiar causal relationships to novel ones, using the features of prospective objects as a guide. If you need to pound a nail but cannot find a hammer, you might pick up a nearby brick instead, reasoning that it will “do the job”; a child who has recently discovered drawing with colored chalks on paper may explore the extent of this new power, using them to draw on the walls, the mirror, or even her bed sheets. These kinds of everyday actions call upon what we call object-based causal generalization.

Results
Discussion
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.