Abstract

Our attitudes/beliefs typically develop gradually, with information appearing over time. This study considered how 6- and 9-year-olds (N = 80) form beliefs from serial information, and how information order affects this, in parallel social and physical judgment tasks. Children updated their beliefs continuously, after each bit of information, or gave one judgment at the end of the series. Updating results showed strong, short-term recency effects; stable beliefs, reflecting all informers, developed as well. These stable beliefs were weaker for younger children; the recency was stronger. Both ages used a running average strategy when serially updating judgments, but a memory-based approach when responding only at the end. The latter produced no recency or age differences and led to stronger beliefs. It is concluded that children use the same serial judgment strategies as adults. Process parameters, e.g., recency weights, change with development/information complexity, but even young children form serial beliefs effectively.

Highlights

  • Our attitudes/beliefs typically develop gradually, with information appearing over time

  • The present study considers two judgment domains, both extensively studied with adults, person impression formation, and non-social judgment of a physical proportion

  • I tell you about some more cities/children, but this time you only make one guess at the very end, after we have looked at several streets/months.”

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Our attitudes/beliefs typically develop gradually, with information appearing over time. This study considered how 6- and 9-year-olds (N = 80) form beliefs from serial information, and how information order affects this, in parallel social and physical judgment tasks Children updated their beliefs continuously, after each bit of information, or gave one judgment at the end of the series. There are large literatures on how adults form beliefs from sequential information, both work on attitude formation/change (e.g., Ajzen, 2005; Albarracín, Johnson, & Zanna, 2014; Petty & Briñol, 2010) and on order effects (Anderson, 1981; Hogarth & Einhorn, 1992; Hovland, 1957). Order effects are pragmatically important: They are ubiquitous, often large, and appear in, for instance, persuasive communication (Petty, Tormala, Hawkins, & Wegener, 2001), responsibility attribution (Gerstenberg & Lagnado, 2012); category induction (Duffy & Crawford, 2008), affective evaluation (Zauberman, Diehl, & Ariely, 2006); legal decision-making (Pennington & Hastie, 1992), auditing (Trotman & Wright, 2000), or political candidate evaluation (McGraw, Lodge, & Stroh, 1990)

Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.