Abstract

BackgroundA low replication rate has been reported in some scientific areas motivating the creation of resource intensive collaborations to estimate the replication rate by repeating individual studies. The substantial resources required by these projects limits the number of studies that can be repeated and consequently the generalizability of the findings. We extend the use of a method from Jager and Leek to estimate the false discovery rate for 94 journals over a 5-year period using p values from over 30,000 abstracts enabling the study of how the false discovery rate varies by journal characteristics.ResultsWe find that the empirical false discovery rate is higher for cancer versus general medicine journals (p = 9.801E−07, 95% CI: 0.045, 0.097; adjusted mean false discovery rate cancer = 0.264 vs. general medicine = 0.194). We also find that false discovery rate is negatively associated with log journal impact factor. A two-fold decrease in journal impact factor is associated with an average increase of 0.020 in FDR (p = 2.545E−04). Conversely, we find no statistically significant evidence of a higher false discovery rate, on average, for Open Access versus closed access journals (p = 0.320, 95% CI − 0.015, 0.046, adjusted mean false discovery rate Open Access = 0.241 vs. closed access = 0.225).ConclusionsOur results identify areas of research that may need additional scrutiny and support to facilitate replicable science. Given our publicly available R code and data, others can complete a broad assessment of the empirical false discovery rate across other subject areas and characteristics of published research.

Highlights

  • A low replication rate has been reported in some scientific areas motivating the creation of resource intensive collaborations to estimate the replication rate by repeating individual studies

  • We evaluate if and how the empirical false discovery rate varies by three journal characteristics: (1) subject area—cancer versus general medicine; (2) 2-year journal impact factor (JIF), and (3) Open Access versus closed access

  • The number of journals by subject area and Open Access status included in the final model is in Table 1.A full list of journals and descriptive information is included in Additional file 1: Table S6

Read more

Summary

Introduction

A low replication rate has been reported in some scientific areas motivating the creation of resource intensive collaborations to estimate the replication rate by repeating individual studies. Efforts have been made to estimate the replication rate by forming large-scale collaborations to repeat a set of published studies within a particular discipline such as psychology [6], cancer biology [14], economics [15], and social sciences [16, 17]. The proportion of studies that replicate vary from approximately 1/3 to 2/3 depending, in part, on the power of the replication studies, the criteria used to define replication, and the proportion of true discoveries in the original set of studies [18] These replication projects are often massive undertakings necessitating a large amount of resources and scientists. The Cancer-Biology Reproducibility project lowered its projected number of studies for replication from 50 to 37 and again to 18 [19]. This suggests that an efficient, complementary approach to evaluate replicability would be highly beneficial

Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call