Abstract

When we treat an animal’s welfare as an individual experience, we should consider the possibility that it may be associated with individual differences in personality. We tested for such associations in 44 socially housed rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) that lived at the California National Primate Research Center. For each macaque, we obtained ratings on a 16-item welfare questionnaire, a 4-item subjective well-being questionnaire, and a 54-item personality questionnaire, the last of which we used to define each macaque’s standing on six personality domains---Confidence, Openness, Dominance, Friendliness, Activity, and Anxiety---identified in an earlier study. Finally, we used focal animal sampling to obtain measures of behavior. We found evidence for interrater agreement for all the welfare items, all but one item from the subjective well-being questionnaire, and all but four items from the personality questionnaire. Using principal components analysis, we found the welfare and subjective well-being survey items loaded together onto a single component. Macaques higher on this dimension received less aggression, engaged in fewer displacement activities (e.g., scratching), and were rated as higher in Confidence, Openness, Dominance, and Friendliness. These results are consistent with reports on chimpanzees and brown capuchin monkeys and constitute further evidence that observer ratings are based on observed behavioral states, suggesting them to be a psychometrically valid way to assess primate welfare.

Highlights

  • In humans, the positive associations between subjective well-being and personality traits related to sociability (e.g., Extraversion) and negative associations between subjective well-being and traits related to anxiety (e.g., Neuroticism) are well-established (DeNeve and Cooper, 1998; Steel et al, 2008)

  • As in human studies, the work on subjective well-being in nonhuman primates has shown that it is stable over time (King and Landau, 2003; Weiss et al, 2011b), heritable (Adams et al, 2012; Weiss et al, 2002), and, at least in orangutans, associated with longer life (Weiss et al, 2011a)

  • We addressed similar questions about welfare in rhesus macaques Macaca mulatta housed at the California National Primate Research Center (CNPRC)

Read more

Summary

Introduction

The positive associations between subjective well-being and personality traits related to sociability (e.g., Extraversion) and negative associations between subjective well-being and traits related to anxiety (e.g., Neuroticism) are well-established (DeNeve and Cooper, 1998; Steel et al, 2008). To study this in nonhuman primates, King and Landau (2003) devised the subjective well-being questionnaire, which they modeled on human subjective well-being scales (Diener and Emmons, 1984; Diener et al, 1999; Sandvik et al, 1993). Welfare surveys have the potential to be an affordable and fast addition to animal welfare methodologies (Whitham and Wielebnowski, 2009) as animal facilities have limited resources (e.g., staff time) to perform traditional forms of welfare assessment

Objectives
Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call