Abstract

Introduction Selection for behavior has played a key role in the history of dog domestication and breeding. Early dog domestication probably involved selection for tameness. A few generations of selection for tameness in the famous Russian silver fox experiments led to a domesticated strain of foxes that showed dog-like behavior and displayed curly tails, drop ears, and loss of pigment (Trut et al. , 2009). The early domestication of dogs was followed by the formation of dog breeds. Different aspects of ancestral wolf behavior have been selected for in different breeds: dogs have been bred to guard, herd, hunt, pull sleds, and to provide companionship. Selection for physical appearance became more important at a later stage when people began breeding dogs for show. Extreme population bottlenecks, founder effects, drift and strong artificial selection for desired traits during breed formation have resulted in a dog population that is a collection of genetic isolates with highly diverging morphology, disease susceptibility, and behavioral characteristics (Sutter & Ostrander, 2004). Scott and Fuller (1965) performed a pioneering study of breed differences and inheritance of canine behavior. Their experiment involved dogs of five breeds: basenji, beagle, American cocker spaniel, Shetland sheepdog and wire-haired fox terrier. Breeds and their crosses were compared for reactivity, trainability and problem-solving behaviors. Scott and Fuller observed behavioral differences between the breeds in the majority of their behavioral tests; for instance, in playful aggression and dominance. Wire-haired fox terriers were the most aggressive, consistently “ganging up” on group members. These attacks were so serious that victims had to be removed in order to prevent serious injury. More recently, Svartberg (2006) compared the behavior of 31 dog breeds using data from a standard behavioral test. Significant breed differences were observed for all investigated traits (Figure 5.1). Dog breeds also differ in the prevalence of problem behavior. For instance, certain breeds are predisposed to obsessive-compulsive behaviors: bull terriers frequently exhibit tail chasing, while Doberman pinschers are prone to acral licking. The fact that breed differences in behavior exist, and that behavioral dispositions can be selected for, suggests that there is a genetic basis for behavior. Behavioral genetics is the study of the individual variation in behavior due to genetic differences between animals. Behavioral genetic studies in dogs have traditionally been studies of breed differences, selection studies, and population-based heritability studies.

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.