Abstract

Severe traumatic brain injury impairs arousal and awareness, the two components of consciousness. Accurate diagnosis of a patient’s level of consciousness is critical for determining treatment goals, access to rehabilitative services, and prognosis. The bedside behavioral examination, the current clinical standard for diagnosis of disorders of consciousness, is prone to misdiagnosis, a finding that has led to the development of advanced neuroimaging techniques aimed at detection of conscious awareness. Although a variety of paradigms have been used in functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to reveal covert consciousness, the relative accuracy of these paradigms in the patient population is unknown. Here, we compare the rate of covert consciousness detection by hand squeezing and tennis playing motor imagery paradigms in 10 patients with traumatic disorders of consciousness [six male, six acute, mean ± SD age = 27.9 ± 9.1 years, one coma, four unresponsive wakefulness syndrome, two minimally conscious without language function, and three minimally conscious with language function, per bedside examination with the Coma Recovery Scale-Revised (CRS-R)]. We also tested the same paradigms in 10 healthy subjects (nine male, mean ± SD age = 28.5 ± 9.4 years). In healthy subjects, the hand squeezing paradigm detected covert command following in 7/10 and the tennis playing paradigm in 9/10 subjects. In patients who followed commands on the CRS-R, the hand squeezing paradigm detected covert command following in 2/3 and the tennis playing paradigm in 0/3 subjects. In patients who did not follow commands on the CRS-R, the hand squeezing paradigm detected command following in 1/7 and the tennis playing paradigm in 2/7 subjects. The sensitivity, specificity, and accuracy (ACC) of detecting covert command following in patients who demonstrated this behavior on the CRS-R was 66.7, 85.7, and 80% for the hand squeezing paradigm and 0, 71.4, and 50% for the tennis playing paradigm, respectively. Overall, the tennis paradigm performed better than the hand squeezing paradigm in healthy subjects, but in patients, the hand squeezing paradigm detected command following with greater ACC. These findings indicate that current fMRI motor imagery paradigms frequently fail to detect command following and highlight the need for paradigm optimization to improve the accuracy of covert consciousness detection.

Highlights

  • Patients with severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) experience a period of impaired consciousness characterized by disturbances in arousal and awareness

  • Hand squeeze and tennis playing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) data were acquired in a convenience sample of 12 patients

  • Precise assessment of level of consciousness (LoC) in patients with severe TBI is critical for guiding clinical management, providing accurate prognoses, and gaining access to rehabilitative services

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Patients with severe traumatic brain injury (TBI) experience a period of impaired consciousness characterized by disturbances in arousal and awareness. This disorder of consciousness (DoC) may resolve acutely [i.e., in the intensive care unit (ICU)] or may be prolonged, extending weeks, months, or even years postinjury [1]. Accurate assessment of a patient’s level of consciousness (LoC) is critically important to prognosis, as patients who have recovered consciousness (i.e., MCS) and especially language function have a higher likelihood of regaining cognitive function than those who have not (i.e., coma and UWS) [7,8,9,10]. An inaccurate diagnosis may prevent autonomous decision-making in patients who retain the capacity to do so

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.