Abstract
Eyetracking technology has had limited application in the dementia field to date, with most studies attempting to discriminate syndrome subgroups on the basis of basic oculomotor functions rather than higher-order cognitive abilities. Eyetracking-based tasks may also offer opportunities to reduce or ameliorate problems associated with standard paper-and-pencil cognitive tests such as the complexity and linguistic demands of verbal test instructions, and the problems of tiredness and attention associated with lengthy tasks that generate few data points at a slow rate. In the present paper we adapted the Brixton spatial anticipation test to a computerized instruction-less version where oculomotor metrics, rather than overt verbal responses, were taken into account as indicators of high level cognitive functions. Twelve bvFTD (in whom spatial anticipation deficits were expected), six SD patients (in whom deficits were predicted to be less frequent) and 38 healthy controls were presented with a 10 × 7 matrix of white circles. During each trial (N = 24) a black dot moved across seven positions on the screen, following 12 different patterns. Participants’ eye movements were recorded. Frequentist statistical analysis of standard eye movement metrics were complemented by a Bayesian machine learning (ML) approach in which raw eyetracking time series datasets were examined to explore the ability to discriminate diagnostic group performance not only on the overall performance but also on individual trials. The original pen and paper Brixton test identified a spatial anticipation deficit in 7/12 (58%) of bvFTD and in 2/6 (33%) of SD patients. The eyetracking frequentist approach reported the deficit in 11/12 (92%) of bvFTD and in none (0%) of the SD patients. The machine learning approach had the main advantage of identifying significant differences from controls in 24/24 individual trials for bvFTD patients and in only 12/24 for SD patients. Results indicate that the fine grained rich datasets obtained from eyetracking metrics can inform us about high level cognitive functions in dementia, such as spatial anticipation. The ML approach can help identify conditions where subtle deficits are present and, potentially, contribute to test optimisation and the reduction of testing times. The absence of instructions also favoured a better distinction between different clinical groups of patients and can help provide valuable disease-specific markers.
Highlights
Eye movement investigations have provided important insights into the study of neurodegenerative conditions and in the discrimination between normal aging processes and abnormal patterns associated with dementia
Results indicated that both behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) and semantic dementia (SD) patients showed normal performance in terms of primary visually guided saccades, as revealed by saccade latencies, time to fixate the first target in the sequence after its onset, fixation duration and saccade velocity
BvFTD but not SD patients made a smaller number of correct anticipatory saccades than controls in zigzag and displaced zigzag conditions
Summary
Eye movement investigations have provided important insights into the study of neurodegenerative conditions and in the discrimination between normal aging processes and abnormal patterns associated with dementia. Eye movement pattern changes in normal aging include a reduced ability to suppress reflexive saccades, a decline in pursuit gain, increased latency, decreased degree and velocity of vergence movements and saccadic intrusions during steady fixation tasks (for a review, see Pelak, 2010). Eyetracking investigations in people with dementia have identified abnormalities in oculomotor characteristics that are distinct from the changes seen in normal aging (e.g., Shakespeare et al, 2015). Patients with dementia have been shown to exhibit deficits in various eye movement measures. Patients with semantic dementia (SD), mainly characterized by anomia and a single word comprehension deficit (Gorno-Tempini et al., 2011), typically show eyetracking metrics comparable to that of controls (Garbutt et al, 2007) Patients with Alzheimer's disease are impaired in the pro-saccade task (fixate a target appearing on the screen; Fletcher and Sharpe, 1986; Bylsma et al, 1995; Yang et al, 2011; Yang et al, 2013) and antisaccade task (look in the opposite direction to that in which the target appeared; Abel et al, 2012; Crawford et al, 2005; Shafiq-Antonacci et al, 2003).
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.