Abstract

Advantages in diverse aspects of cognitive functioning have been reported in early bilinguals (Bialystok, 2011) as well as in children frequenting an early bilingual immersion school program (Nicolay and Poncelet, 2015). However, during the last decade, some studies failed to replicate these advantages. Currently, the presence of cognitive benefits in children frequenting an immersion program remains debated. The lack of consistency between the studies could come from the fact that time spent by children within the immersion program is variable from one study to the other and that studies used different tasks to assess the same cognitive function. The main aim of the present study was to determine how time spent in immersion affects the emergence of cognitive advantages along the primary schooling. We compared 196 immersed Dutch-speaking children since they were 5 years old and 195 non-immersed French-speaking children, from different grades of the primary schooling (i.e., at 6, 7, 8, and 12 years old) by using the same attentional and executive tasks as those used in previous studies having shown a bilingual advantage. Furthermore, these groups were matched on a set of variables known to influence cognitive functioning. After 1, 2, and 3 years of enrolment in this program, performances of immersed compared to non-immersed children did not differ for any task. However, after 6 years, immersed children outperformed non-immersed children on the cognitive flexibility and the working memory tasks. These results show that, in French-speaking children immersed in Dutch, cognitive advantages could depend on the length of time spent in immersion since they are not present at the beginning (after 1, 2, and 3 years) but seem to emerge at the end of it (after 6 years). In contrast, in previous studies conducted in English immersion, advantages appear at the beginning of the primary schooling but are absent at the end of it. Furthermore, these results suggest that the emergence of cognitive advantages may vary depending on the second language learned. The results are discussed in terms of linguistic characteristics and status of the languages at stake.

Highlights

  • A large number of studies have shown that early bilingualism can positively affect cognitive functions such as inhibition and cognitive flexibility (Bialystok et al, 2004; Bialystok et al, 2008; Costa et al, 2008; Adesope et al, 2010; Prior and MacWhinney, 2010; Antón et al, 2019), attentional abilities (e.g., Chung-FatYim et al, 2016), and working memory (e.g., Blom et al, 2014)

  • Concerning the three first grades, a series of two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted to determine whether a difference would be found with school progression according to the group in the different cognitive measures

  • Concerning sixth grade, because the two groups were not matched on gender, we used a series of unique ANCOVA to determine whether a difference would be found according to the group in terms of the different cognitive measures applied

Read more

Summary

Introduction

A large number of studies have shown that early bilingualism can positively affect cognitive functions such as inhibition and cognitive flexibility (Bialystok et al, 2004; Bialystok et al, 2008; Costa et al, 2008; Adesope et al, 2010; Prior and MacWhinney, 2010; Antón et al, 2019), attentional abilities (e.g., Chung-FatYim et al, 2016), and working memory (e.g., Blom et al, 2014) These advantages were revealed in immersion education programs such as the Content and Language Integrated Learning (CLIL) program where children are exposed to a second language (L2) early (for example, as soon as third kindergarten) and massively, between 50 and 75% of school time.

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call