Abstract

The ability to learn and store information should be adapted to the environment in which animals operate to confer a selective advantage. Yet the relationship between learning, memory, and the environment is poorly understood, and further complicated by phenotypic plasticity caused by the very environment in which learning and memory need to operate. Many insect species show polyphenism, an extreme form of phenotypic plasticity, allowing them to occupy distinct environments by producing two or more alternative phenotypes. Yet how the learning and memories capabilities of these alternative phenotypes are adapted to their specific environments remains unknown for most polyphenic insect species. The desert locust can exist as one of two extreme phenotypes or phases, solitarious and gregarious. Recent studies of associative food-odor learning in this locust have shown that aversive but not appetitive learning differs between phases. Furthermore, switching from the solitarious to the gregarious phase (gregarization) prevents locusts acquiring new learned aversions, enabling them to convert an aversive memory formed in the solitarious phase to an appetitive one in the gregarious phase. This conversion provides a neuroecological mechanism that matches key changes in the behavioral environments of the two phases. These findings emphasize the importance of understanding the neural mechanisms that generate ecologically relevant behaviors and the interactions between different forms of behavioral plasticity.

Highlights

  • Insects, like all animals, learn and store new information about their environment, and use it to modify various behaviors including feeding, predator avoidance, social interaction, and sexual behavior (Papaj and Prokopy 1989; Dukas 2008a; Chittka and Niven 2009)

  • Polyphenism is an extreme form of phenotypic plasticity, which is intrinsic to all organisms and biological processes (WestEberhard 2003; Pigliucci et al 2006; Fusco and Minelli 2010; Moczek 2010)

  • High conditioned proboscis extension reflex (PER) rates are seen in other insect species with a proboscis, such as moths (Hartlieb 1996; Fan et al 1997) and fruit flies (Chabaud et al 2006), exceptions do exist

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Like all animals, learn and store new information about their environment, and use it to modify various behaviors including feeding, predator avoidance, social interaction, and sexual behavior (Papaj and Prokopy 1989; Dukas 2008a; Chittka and Niven 2009). This phase difference in aversive learning is specific to the acquisition but not to the retention or retrieval of long-term memories (Simoes et al 2013): When an unfamiliar odor is paired with NHT-diet, solitarious locusts manifest conditioned aversion to the odor within 10 min after training.

Results
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.