Abstract

The first aim of the present study was to test whether arousing, aversive sounds can influence inhibitory task performance and lead to increased error monitoring relative to a neutral task condition. The second aim was to examine whether the enhancement of error monitoring in an affective context (if present) could be predicted from stop-signal-related brain activity. Participants performed an emotional stop-signal task that required response inhibition to aversive and neutral auditory stimuli. The behavioral data revealed that unpleasant sounds facilitated inhibitory processing by decreasing the stop-signal reaction time and increasing the inhibitory rate relative to neutral tones. Aversive sounds evoked larger N1, P3, and Pe components, indicating improvements in perceptual processing, inhibition, and conscious error monitoring. A first regression analysis, conducted regardless of the category of the stop signal, revealed that both selected indexes of stop-signal-related brain activity—the N1 and P3 amplitudes recorded in the unsuccessfully inhibited trials—significantly accounted for the Pe component variance, explaining a large amount of the observed variation (66%). A second regression model, focused on difference measures (emotional minus neutral), revealed that the affective increase in the P3 amplitude on failed stop trials was the only factor that significantly accounted for the emotional enhancement effect in the Pe amplitude. This suggests that, in general (regardless of stop-signal condition), error processing is stronger if the erroneous response directly follows the stimulus, which was effectively processed on both the perceptual and action-monitoring levels. However, only inhibition-monitoring evidence accounts for the emotional increase in conscious error detection.

Highlights

  • The first aim of the present study was to test whether arousing, aversive sounds can influence inhibitory task performance and lead to increased error monitoring relative to a neutral task condition

  • The stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) was significantly shorter in the emotional condition (M = 203.3 ms, SD = 23.3) than in the neutral condition (M = 217.3 ms, SD = 23.3), which indicates that participants were better at inhibiting the responses with emotional stop signals than neutral stop signals, t(31) = 6.10, p < .001, d = 0.6

  • This study investigated, first, whether task-relevant, unpleasant, arousing sounds can modulate task performance and lead to increased error-monitoring activity relative to a neutral task condition, and second, whether the emotional enhancement effect on performance monitoring could be predicted from the stop-signal-related brain activity observed in the unsuccessfully inhibited trials

Read more

Summary

Introduction

The first aim of the present study was to test whether arousing, aversive sounds can influence inhibitory task performance and lead to increased error monitoring relative to a neutral task condition. A second regression model, focused on difference measures (emotional minus neutral), revealed that the affective increase in the P3 amplitude on failed stop trials was the only factor that significantly accounted for the emotional enhancement effect in the Pe amplitude This suggests that, in general (regardless of stopsignal condition), error processing is stronger if the erroneous response directly follows the stimulus, which was effectively processed on both the perceptual and action-monitoring. Wiswede and colleagues noticed that unpleasant pictures presented 700 ms prior to flanker stimuli increased the size of the ERN relative to neutral or pleasant pictures (Wiswede, Münte, Goschke, & Russeler, 2009) In both studies, task-irrelevant emotional stimuli were used to induce an affective state. Scattered evidence suggests that short-duration affective states induction may influence Pe amplitude (Moser et al, 2005; Paul et al, 2017)

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.