Abstract
Astronauts exposed to microgravity face sensorimotor challenges affecting balance control when readapting to Earth's gravity upon return from spaceflight. Small amounts of electrical noise applied to the vestibular system have been shown to improve balance control during standing and walking under discordant sensory conditions in healthy subjects, likely by enhancing information transfer through the phenomenon of stochastic resonance. The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that imperceptible levels of stochastic vestibular stimulation (SVS) could improve short-term adaptation to a locomotor task in a novel sensory discordant environment. Healthy subjects (14 males, 10 females, age = 28.7 ± 5.3 years, height = 167.2 ± 9.6 cm, weight = 71.0 ± 12.8 kg) were tested for perceptual thresholds to sinusoidal currents applied across the mastoids. Subjects were then randomly and blindly assigned to an SVS group receiving a 0–30 Hz Gaussian white noise electrical stimulus at 50% of their perceptual threshold (stim) or a control group receiving zero stimulation during Functional Mobility Tests (FMTs), nine trials of which were done under conditions of visual discordance (wearing up/down vision reversing goggles). Time to complete the course (TCC) was used to test the effect of SVS between the two groups across the trials. Adaptation rates from the normalized TCCs were also compared utilizing exponent values of power fit trendline equations. A one-tailed independent-samples t-test indicated these adaptation rates were significantly faster in the stim group (n = 12) than the control (n = 12) group [t(16.18) = 2.00, p = 0.031]. When a secondary analysis was performed comparing “responders” (subjects who showed faster adaptation rates) of the stim (n = 7) group to the control group (n = 12), independent-samples t-tests revealed significantly faster trial times for the last five trials with goggles in the stim group “responders” than the controls. The data suggests that SVS may be capable of improving short-term adaptation to a locomotion task done under sensory discordance in a group of responsive subjects.
Highlights
In astronauts, prolonged exposure to microgravity induces an adaptation to that environment resulting in reinterpretation of visual, vestibular, and somatosensory inputs (Paloski et al, 1992, 1994; Reschke et al, 1994; Bloomberg et al, 2015)
The initial one-tailed independent-samples t-tests revealed significantly faster adaptation rates (α) in the stim (n = 12) group than the control (n = 12) group [t(16.18) = 2.00, p = 0.031], but no significant differences were found between the two groups for any of the normalized TCC times completed during visual discordance
We investigated if stochastic vestibular stimulation (SVS) could improve locomotor performance within an adaptation paradigm
Summary
In astronauts, prolonged exposure to microgravity induces an adaptation to that environment resulting in reinterpretation of visual, vestibular, and somatosensory inputs (Paloski et al, 1992, 1994; Reschke et al, 1994; Bloomberg et al, 2015). The processes by which these rapid and slower adaptation curves occur are often referred to as strategic control and adaptive realignment respectively (Redding and Wallace, 2002; Richards et al, 2007; Mulavara et al, 2010) These two processes are considered interdependent (Redding and Wallace, 2002; Richards et al, 2007), and longer-term locomotor adaptive recovery has been shown to be associated with short-term strategic capabilities of astronauts readapting from long-duration spaceflight. One proposed countermeasure, which has been shown to benefit sensory system capabilities and associated performance improvements, is through the addition of small amounts of noise via a phenomenon known as stochastic resonance (SR)
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.