Abstract
Prospective memory (PM) refers to our ability to perform actions at the appropriate moment, either when a predetermined event occurs (event-based, EB) or after a predetermined amount of time (time-based, TB). Sleep favors the consolidation of both EB and TB intentions, but whether this benefit is preserved during ageing is still subject to debate. PM was assessed in 28 young and 27 older healthy volunteers using a virtual environment. Participants had to learn and execute intentions after intervals filled with either daytime wakefulness or nighttime sleep. Intentions consisted of four TB, four EB with a strong link between the cue triggering retrieval and the action to be performed (EB-link) and four with no link (EB-nolink). PM was not affected by age, whatever the type of intention and the nature of the retention interval. While sleep reinforced all types of intentions in young participants, this benefit was only observed for TB and EB-link intentions in older adults. Sleep also reinforced the intrinsic PM components in both groups. Thus, when assessed using complex realistic situations, PM is not impaired in ageing. Results are discussed in the light of memory schema theory and the possible impact of cognitive reserve on sleep and memory.
Highlights
Sleep favors the consolidation of recently acquired information in long-term memory stores [1,2]
The prospective component involves remembering that something has to be done at the appropriate moment, while the retrospective component refers to the content of the intention
Comparisons of sleep parameters obtained during the post-learning night showed that there were no group differences in terms of total sleep time (TST), sleep onset latency (SOL), wake after sleep onset (WASO), SE, or sleep fragmentation
Summary
Sleep favors the consolidation of recently acquired information in long-term memory stores [1,2]. The retrieval of an intention can be triggered either by the occurrence of an external event, namely the prospective cue (e.g., “take the cake out of the oven when the timer rings”) or after a defined amount of time (e.g., “take the cake out of the oven after 30 minutes”). These two forms of PM refer respectively to event-based (EB) and time-based (TB) PM [16]. While these intentions are held in mind, individuals engage in other activities (named the ongoing task), distinguishing PM from simple planning
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.