Abstract
Referential alarm calls that denote specific types of dangers are common across diverse vertebrate lineages. Different alarm calls can indicate a variety of threats, which often require specific actions to evade. Thus, to benefit from the call, listeners of referential alarm calls must be able to decode the signaled threat and respond to it in an appropriate manner. Yellow warblers (Setophaga petechia) produce referential “seet” calls that signal to conspecifics the presence of nearby obligate brood parasitic brown-headed cowbirds (Molothrus ater), which lay their eggs in the nests of other species, including yellow warblers. Our previous playback experiments have found that red-winged blackbirds (Agelaius phoeniceus), a species also parasitized by brown-headed cowbirds, eavesdrop upon and respond strongly to yellow warbler seet calls during the incubation stage of breeding with aggression similar to responses to both cowbird chatters and predator calls. To assess whether red-winged blackbird responses to seet calls vary with their own risk of brood parasitism, we presented the same playbacks during the nestling stage of breeding (when the risk of brood parasitism is lower than during incubation). As predicted, we found that blackbirds mediated their aggression toward both cowbird chatter calls and the warblers’ anti-parasitic referential alarm calls in parallel with the low current risk of brood parasitism during the nestling stage. These results further support that red-winged blackbirds flexibly respond to yellow warbler antiparasitic referential calls as a frontline defense against brood parasitism at their own nests.
Highlights
Acoustic signals are used across diverse lineages to signal a variety of information, such as food sources or predatory threats (Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011)
Both types of listeners react to aerial alarm calls by hiding under cover, but, critically, the hornbills do not respond to the alarm calls signaling ground predators because they do not pose a threat to these birds
Blackbirds are not known to have a referential alarm call system of their own, but they do eavesdrop upon the seet calls of yellow warbler neighbors: in Lawson et al (2020) we found that during the incubation stage, blackbirds of both sexes responded more often to the warblers’ seet calls relative to their generic “chip” alarm calls, and with similar urgency and vocal aggression toward playbacks of seet calls as to both cowbird chatters and nest predator calls
Summary
Acoustic signals are used across diverse lineages to signal a variety of information, such as food sources or predatory threats (Bradbury and Vehrencamp, 2011). Verreaux’s sifakas (Propithecus verreauxi) produce different referential alarm calls for aerial vs terrestrial predators, which are heard by both intended conspecific receivers and by eavesdropping heterospecific black-casqued hornbills (Ceratogymna atrata) (Rainey et al, 2004a,b) Both types of listeners react to aerial alarm calls by hiding under cover, but, critically, the hornbills do not respond to the alarm calls signaling ground predators because they do not pose a threat to these birds. Yellow warblers themselves respond less aggressively to referential seet calls during the nestling stage likely because there is little to no brood parasitism risk during this stage (Neudorf and Sealy, 1992; Gill and Sealy, 1996), and the same pattern can be seen across other hosts toward models of their respective brood parasites (Fasanella and Fernández, 2009; reviewed in Lawson et al, 2021a). We predicted that if blackbirds respond to seet calls as a referent for brood parasites, aggressive responses toward cowbird chatter and seet calls should be lower than aggressive responses to nest predator calls, but comparable to each other, during the later stage of nesting
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.