Abstract

When we see an object, we know where it is. Or do we? Perhaps not in indirect vision, as was observed by the gestalt psychologist Korte in 1923. Objects and object parts appear to 'dance around', and these phenomena may underlie a part of what is called the crowding effect today. From Korte's account of pattern recognition in indirect vision, I select two phenomena: a loss of the positional code for letter parts and a loss of the same for whole letters. Using these examples, I present a novel, speculative explanation for a contradiction in the literature: patterns located more peripherally than a target show more interference than do more centrally located patterns, yet for whole-letter confusions the asymmetry is the other way round. The inward, not the outward, flanker is increasingly confused with a target at increasing target eccentricities. I propose that feature-binding decreases with eccentricity such that free-floating letter parts more often intrude from the periphery and whole letters from the centre. I conclude with a few remarks on computational modelling as, hopefully, a challenge to neural computationalists.

Highlights

  • Visual crowding is the phenomenon that a pattern is less well recognized in the presence of neighbouring patterns than when seen in isolation.(1) It is a rather strong effect in more than 99.9% of the visual field, (2) and it happens every moment in everyday life

  • I propose that feature-binding decreases with eccentricity such that free-floating letter parts more often intrude from the periphery and whole letters from the centre

  • 1 Introduction Visual crowding is the phenomenon that a pattern is less well recognized in the presence of neighbouring patterns than when seen in isolation.(1) It is a rather strong effect in more than 99.9% of the visual field, (2) and it happens every moment in everyday life

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Visual crowding is the phenomenon that a pattern is less well recognized in the presence of neighbouring patterns than when seen in isolation.(1) It is a rather strong effect in more than 99.9% of the visual field, (2) and it happens every moment in everyday life (when the eyes are open). Visual spatial attention is mostly focused on the fovea, so much so that eye movements are called overt attention, and we seem to confuse the phenomenal characteristics of foveal visual perception with visual perception in general. As another example, contours in peripheral vision appear sharp (3) (cf Galvin, O’Shea, Squire, & Govan, 1997; Galvin, O’Shea, Squire, & Hailstone, 1999), even though acuity, which would be thought to be at the basis of perceived sharpness, is drastically reduced there

H Strasburger
Findings
Modelling crowding
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.