Abstract
Creative mindsets reflect the implicit beliefs individuals hold regarding the nature of creativity as innate (i.e., fixed mindset) or malleable (i.e., growth mindset). Karwowski (2014) developed the Creative Mindsets Scale (CMS), in which fixed and growth creative mindsets were each measured with five items. Across three studies, the current study aimed to examine its psychometric properties in Chinese settings and to explore to what extent effects of creative mindsets on creativity were generalized to the real workplace. Based on the survey data of 216 college students (Study 1) and 205 full-time employees (Study 2) in China, results consistently indicated that a two-factor structure, in which both types of creative mindsets were independent of each other, was confirmed. Measures of both types of creative mindsets were of satisfactory psychometric features in terms of reliability (internal consistency) and validity (construct, convergent, and discriminant validities). Furthermore, Study 1 provided evidence for the incremental validity of creative mindsets beyond mindsets of intelligence in explaining creative personal identity and creative self-efficacy. Based on a third independent sample consisting of 282 full-time employees from several Chinese companies, Study 3 further demonstrated that measures of creative mindsets could predict employees’ creative performance as rated by their supervisors, lending additional support for their generalizability to the real workplace. Moreover, growth mindset, but not fixed mindset, was significantly related to creative performance, and such an effect was mediated by effort. The present study contributes to the creative mindset literature by cross-validating the CMS’s psychometric properties in a new setting and empirically establishing the link between creative mindsets and employees’ creativity in the real workplace.
Highlights
It’s long been documented that people hold different beliefs about the malleability of human general attributes such as intelligence (Dweck, 1999; Blackwell et al, 2007), personality (Chiu et al, 1997b), morality (Chiu et al, 1997a), and even many specific attributes like willpower (Job et al, 2010), interest (O’Keefe et al, 2018), or emotion (Tamir et al, 2007)
We compared the fitness of the hypothesized model with the alternative one in terms of Comparative Fit Index (CFI), Tucker Lewis Index (TLI), Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA), and Standardized Root Mean Square Residual (SRMR)
Based on confirmative factor analysis (CFA) results, factor structure was first examined, and convergent and discriminant validity were further assessed according to composite reliability (CR) and average variance extracted (AVE) (Fornell and Larcker, 1981; Raykov, 1997)
Summary
It’s long been documented that people hold different beliefs about the malleability of human general attributes such as intelligence (Dweck, 1999; Blackwell et al, 2007), personality (Chiu et al, 1997b), morality (Chiu et al, 1997a), and even many specific attributes like willpower (Job et al, 2010), interest (O’Keefe et al, 2018), or emotion (Tamir et al, 2007) These beliefs are the manifestation of their implicit theories or mindsets. Individuals holding the growth mindset tend to adopt learning goals rather than performance goals (Burnette et al, 2013), exert more efforts to achieve goals (Schumann et al, 2014), are more resilient and persistent while facing setbacks (Aronson et al, 2002), and recover better from failures (Howe and Dweck, 2016)
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.