Abstract

Automated driving systems (ADS) partially or fully perform or assist with primary driving functions. According to SAE J3016 (SAE, 2016), ADS can subsume driving tasks traditionally reserved for humans, ranging from L0 (no automation) to L5 (full automation), creating varying degrees of driver interaction and responsibility. However, the literature on human-automation interaction indicates that human operators may perform at a suboptimal level when interacting with automated support systems (Parasuraman & Riley, 1997), reducing the net benefit that automation can bring while also simultaneously increasing the potential for unforeseen human errors. Yamani and Horrey (in press) proposed a theoretical framework of human-automation interaction building upon a human information-processing model (Wickens, Hollands, Banbury, & Parasuraman, 2013) that accounts for human performance when interacting with varying types and levels of automation (Parasuraman, Sheridan, & Wickens, 2000). Following the model by Yamani and Horrey (in press), we hypothesized that when the ADS is perceived to be reliable, drivers engaging with such systems (e.g. L2) would exhibit eye movements no better or worse than the drivers engaged with manual or L0 driving since the drivers allocate their reserved or spare resources to other driving-irrelevant activities such as mind wandering or task irrelevant thoughts (Yanko & Spalek, 2014). The current driving simulator study compared young drivers’ eye movements across four unique scenarios in either L0 or L2 driving systems. We asked participants to complete a three-phased skill-based training program (RAPT-3; see Unverricht, Samuel, & Yamani for review) proven effective to improve young drivers’ ability to anticipate latent hazards, immediately followed by the evaluation of their eye movements in either L0 or L2 systems using a head-mounted eye tracker and a driving simulator. Participants in the L2 condition were instructed that the system detects and mitigates existing and latent threats on the forward roadway while maintaining appropriate speed and lateral positioning for the duration of the drive. To ensure similarity between both systems, L2 participants were required to position their hands on the steering wheel and feet above the pedal. No hazards materialized in any of the four driving scenarios. Data showed similar breadths of eye movements for the drivers of the L2 and L0 systems both horizontally [M = 36.5 vs. 36.3 pixels; L2 and L0, respectively] and vertically [M = 26.9 vs. 34.5 pixels] and no difference in mean fixation durations [M = 367 vs. 333 ms for L2 and L0 conditions]. However, data indicated substantial differences between L0 and L2 conditions for number of fixations, with L2 drivers fixating less frequently than L0 drivers, [M = 687 vs. 796 fixations, t (22) = 2.53, B10 = 3.23]. The results imply that L2 drivers may sample information from the forward roadway less often than L0 drivers, suggesting the mobilization of spare resources for non-driving related tasks. Future research should examine the relationship between conveyed system reliability and attention allocation for drivers of ADS with different automation levels. In summary, the current results support Yamani and Horrey’s model and offer potential implications for the design of autonomous systems and the NHTSA automation guidelines to consider the perceived reliability of lower level ADS towards ascribing the role of the driver when the driving task is either partially or fully automated.

Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call