Abstract
Scuttling in sewers and crawling through cracked pavements, cockroaches must search for food in the dark by using their antennae to sniff out decaying plant and animal scraps. Being able to distinguish between very similar smells would help them decide what's edible and what's not. A great deal is known about which chemicals the receptor cells in cockroach antennae respond to, but neurophysiology cannot tell us what odours cockroaches can really distinguish in the wild. Sakura and colleagues tackle this problem by training cockroaches to indicate when they can tell alcoholic fragrances apart. Their findings allow the team to draw links for the first time between what happens in nerve cells and what happens in dirty kitchens after dark.Sakura and team taught cockroaches to choose filter paper soaked in peppermint essence over paper soaked in vanilla by providing a sugary reward for a correct answer and a salty punishment for an error. Testing revealed that cockroaches learn to make preferences very quickly and can retain them for a long time. They also found that these preferences can easily be overridden by training them with different flavours. The team then repeated the training, but substituted three different types of alcohols instead of the peppermint and vanilla. The team knew that if the cockroaches could learn to recognise the alcohol type associated with the reward, it meant that they could distinguish between similar alcohols; and this is exactly what happened. The cockroaches could discriminate a whole range of alcohols, which sometimes differed only by a single carbon atom or by subtleties in the chemical structure. The cockroaches could also tell apart different concentrations of alcohols and mixtures of them – with a few exceptions. Comparisons show that cockroaches' ability to discriminate alcohols is as good as bees and humans!Some of the alcohols used in the study are plant components, such as green leaf odour, lemons or floral fragrances. In the wild, cockroaches would rarely approach these smells, since they do not usually eat living plants. But they can be taught to favour these plant extracts over other alcohols. This reflects the enormous flexibility in their diet, which makes them such successful scavengers.Sakura and colleagues compared their behavioural findings with the information available about cockroach antennae. Antennae contain many receptor cells, which taste the surrounding air. These receptors fall into eight categories, only one of which responds to alcohol. This group of alcohol-sensitive cells can be further divided into subclasses, each responding differently to a range of alcohol types. The team noticed that in some cases, the alcohols discriminated during training fell into different subclasses of alcohol-sensitive cells. In these instances, the behavioural findings were in agreement with the neurophysiology. In other cases, however,the cockroaches could discriminate alcohols that fell into the same receptor subclass. This meant that these alcohols are told apart, not in the antennae but by further processing in the brain. Such relationships between neurophysiology and behaviour advance understanding by tugging at the entangled mesh of mechanisms underlying learning and processing in insect nervous systems.
Highlights
In their first sets of investigations, lobsters were transported from their capture site in closed boxes, some of which had dangling, swinging magnets, along a circuitous route by boat or lorry to a test site
Lobsters captured north–northeast of the test site oriented significantly homeward during the test, while animals captured west–southwest of the test site oriented towards home in a predominantly southwesterly direction
No differences in orientation occurred between animals transported in the presence of constantly swinging magnets, which produced fields strong enough to continuously misalign a compass placed outside the box, and those that did not have magnetic fields disrupted during transport
Summary
Keeping track of the literature isn’t easy, so Outside JEB is a monthly feature that reports the most exciting developments in experimental biology. The spiny lobster is a crustacean known for both its large nocturnal foraging range and long seasonal migrations, and has been shown to exhibit homing behavior if removed from home territories to an unfamiliar location This led Larry Boles and Ken Lohmann, of the University of North Carolina Chapel Hill, to question whether lobsters are capable of true navigation. While unable to show in these initial experiments the precise nature of the ‘map’ and the magnetic features used by these lobsters, Boles and Lohmann have demonstrated for the first time the ability of an invertebrate to derive sufficient information from the Earth’s magnetic fields to determine the location of ‘home’.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.