Abstract

The classification of red meat as “probably carcinogenic” and processed meat as “carcinogenic” was followed by pleas to place warning labels, akin to those used for tobacco products, onto meat products. These labels educate people about the health risks associated with the target behavior and are typically accompanied by graphic imagery that elicits disgust (e.g., a picture of blackened lungs). Although the emotion of disgust has been shown to be an effective tool to affect consumer attitudes toward meat, it remains unclear whether such graphic warning labels that recruit disgust would also affect people's intentions to reduce their meat consumption. Two experiments reveal that graphic warning labels, by recruiting disgust, can increase people's intention to reduce their current levels of meat consumption. However, by eliciting disgust, graphic warning labels can simultaneously trigger reactance: graphic images can make people feel they are being manipulated, thereby ironically decreasing meat-reduction intentions. In a final experiment, we aimed to circumvent reactance by providing disgusting information under the guise of trivia, thereby avoiding the perception that the disgusting information was meant to manipulate. Via this route, disgust becomes a potent tool to influence consumers' intentions to consume meat. Ethical concerns are discussed.

Highlights

  • Meat consumption is on the rise and unlikely to reduce anytime soon without intervention (Cole & McCoskey, 2013)

  • In line with our expectations, we find that graphic warning labels, via recruiting disgust, increase participants’ meat-reduction in­ tentions

  • As in Study 1, there was no difference in intention to reduce the consumption of meat between participants exposed to the graphic warning label (MGraphic = 5.05, SDGraphic = 1.69) and participants exposed to the non-graphic warning label (MNon-graphic = 4.99, SDNongraphic = 1.75; t(1, 470) = 0.415; MDifference = 0.07, Cohen’s d = 0.04, p = .678, 95% CI [− 0.25, 0.38])

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Meat consumption is on the rise and unlikely to reduce anytime soon without intervention (Cole & McCoskey, 2013). Imme­ diately following this classification, the introduction of warning labels akin to those displayed on tobacco products has been advocated by ac­ ademics (Hadi, 2016) and consumer advocacy groups (Center for Sci­ ence in the Public Interest, 2016). Warning labels, like those displayed on tobacco products, educate consumers about the detrimental consequences of engaging in certain behaviors, but typically include graphic images illustrating these consequences, which elicit negative emotions such as fear and disgust (Hammond, 2011). As disgust reliably elicits the singular reaction of avoidance (Morales, Wu, & Fitzsimons, 2012), disgust can increase the persuasiveness of cessation interventions

Objectives
Methods
Results
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.