Abstract

The third driverless car competition of the DARPA Grand Challenge (Urban Challenge) in 2007 saw six autonomous vehicle teams finishing the event successfully. Since then, Automated Vehicles (AVs) made huge strides towards deployment on a large scale. Despite all this progress, AVs continue to make mistakes, some of which have resulted in the deaths of passengers and pedestrians. These crashes received wide coverage in the media and drew a parallel bleak picture on the public’s lack of enthusiasm for this technology. However, not all mistakes are equal. While some mistakes are avoidable, others are hard to avoid even by highly-experienced professional drivers. As they continue to shape citizens’ attitudes towards AVs, we need to understand whether people differentiate between different types of error, and whether these are treated proportionally. In this paper, we ask the following two questions: 1) when an automated car makes a mistake, does the perceived difficulty or novelty of the situation predict blame attributed to it? How does that blame attribution compare to a human driving a regular car? Through two studies we find that the amount of blame people attribute to machine drivers and human drivers is sensitive to the difficulty of the situation. However, while some situations could be more difficult for machine drivers and others are harder for human drivers, people blamed machine drivers more, regardless. Our results provide insights on a crucial, yet under-studied, angle in understanding psychological barriers impeding the public’s adoption of AVs.

Highlights

  • Once properly prepared and finalized to deploy on the roads, automated vehicles (AVs) are expected to bring many benefits, such as decreasing the rate of car crashes (Gao et al, 2014), reducing pollution (Spieser et al, 2014), and increasing traffic efficiency (van Arem et al, 2006)

  • When an Automated vehicles (AVs) makes a mistake, does the perceived difficulty or novelty of the situation predict blame attributed to it? How does that blame attribution compare to a human driving a car? Through two studies, we find that the amount of blame people attribute to AVs and human drivers is sensitive to situation difficulty

  • While some situations could be more difficult for AVs and others for human drivers, people blamed AVs more, regardless

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Once properly prepared and finalized to deploy on the roads, automated vehicles (AVs) are expected to bring many benefits, such as decreasing the rate of car crashes (Gao et al, 2014), reducing pollution (Spieser et al, 2014), and increasing traffic efficiency (van Arem et al, 2006). Assuming that AVs will overcome all remaining technical challenges before they are ready to deliver these benefits, while exhibiting no serious drawbacks, their deployment on a larger scale would be beneficial. These benefits will not be realized if people are not ready to buy them, and various considerations contribute to the public’s aversion to adopting this technology. As argued in (Awad et al, 2020), negative public reaction may result in inflated prices of this technology (Geistfeld, 2017) and may shape how a tort-based regulatory scheme would turn out, both of which can influence the rate of adoption

Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call