Abstract

Some studies have linked bilingualism with a later onset of dementia, Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Not all studies have observed such relationships, however. Differences in study outcomes may be due to methodological limitations and the presence of confounding factors within studies such as immigration status and level of education. We conducted the first systematic review with meta-analysis combining cross-sectional studies to explore if bilingualism might delay symptom onset and diagnosis of dementia, AD, and MCI. Primary outcomes included the age of symptom onset, the age at diagnosis of MCI or dementia, and the risk of developing MCI or dementia. A secondary outcome included the degree of disease severity at dementia diagnosis. There was no difference in the age of MCI diagnosis between monolinguals and bilinguals [mean difference: 3.2; 95% confidence intervals (CI): −3.4, 9.7]. Bilinguals vs. monolinguals reported experiencing AD symptoms 4.7 years (95% CI: 3.3, 6.1) later. Bilinguals vs. monolinguals were diagnosed with dementia 3.3 years (95% CI: 1.7, 4.9) later. Here, 95% prediction intervals showed a large dispersion of effect sizes (−1.9 to 8.5). We investigated this dispersion with a subgroup meta-analysis comparing studies that had recruited participants with dementia to studies that had recruited participants with AD on the age of dementia and AD diagnosis between mono- and bilinguals. Results showed that bilinguals vs. monolinguals were 1.9 years (95% CI: −0.9, 4.7) and 4.2 (95% CI: 2.0, 6.4) older than monolinguals at the time of dementia and AD diagnosis, respectively. The mean difference between the two subgroups was not significant. There was no significant risk reduction (odds ratio: 0.89; 95% CI: 0.68–1.16) in developing dementia among bilinguals vs. monolinguals. Also, there was no significant difference (Hedges’ g = 0.05; 95% CI: −0.13, 0.24) in disease severity at dementia diagnosis between bilinguals and monolinguals, despite bilinguals being significantly older. The majority of studies had adjusted for level of education suggesting that education might not have played a role in the observed delay in dementia among bilinguals vs. monolinguals. Although findings indicated that bilingualism was on average related to a delayed onset of dementia, the magnitude of this relationship varied across different settings. This variation may be due to unexplained heterogeneity and different sources of bias in the included studies. Registration: PROSPERO CRD42015019100.

Highlights

  • RationaleApproximately 43.8 million people lived with dementia worldwide in the year 2016 (Nichols et al, 2019) and this number is projected to increase to 115.5 million people by 2050 (Prince et al, 2013)

  • We assessed whether bilingualism relative to monolingualism might delay the age at which participants experienced the initial symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and delay the age at which participants were diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia

  • In response to previous criticisms (Fuller-Thomson, 2015; Fuller-Thomson & Kuh, 2014; Mukadam et al, 2017), we explored whether immigration status might have been related to differences in the age of dementia diagnosis by conducting a subgroup meta-analysis

Read more

Summary

Introduction

RationaleApproximately 43.8 million people lived with dementia worldwide in the year 2016 (Nichols et al, 2019) and this number is projected to increase to 115.5 million people by 2050 (Prince et al, 2013). A five-year delay in the onset of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the most common form of dementia, could reduce the number of patients living with the disease worldwide by 57%, thereby alleviating the associated economic costs by half (Sperling et al, 2011). Identifying modifiable lifestyle factors that can slow or delay the onset of dementia is a world’s public health priority (WHO, 2017; Wortmann, 2012). One such factor may be bilingualism, which is the ability to speak two languages (Luk & Bialystok, 2013). Some authors have argued that confounding factors including migration status and education may explain some differences in study outcomes in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (FullerThomson, 2015; Fuller-Thomson & Kuh, 2014)

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call