Abstract

BackgroundAir pollution is a suspected developmental neurotoxicant. Many schools are located in close proximity to busy roads, and traffic air pollution peaks when children are at school. We aimed to assess whether exposure of children in primary school to traffic-related air pollutants is associated with impaired cognitive development.Methods and FindingsWe conducted a prospective study of children (n = 2,715, aged 7 to 10 y) from 39 schools in Barcelona (Catalonia, Spain) exposed to high and low traffic-related air pollution, paired by school socioeconomic index; children were tested four times (i.e., to assess the 12-mo developmental trajectories) via computerized tests (n = 10,112). Chronic traffic air pollution (elemental carbon [EC], nitrogen dioxide [NO2], and ultrafine particle number [UFP; 10–700 nm]) was measured twice during 1-wk campaigns both in the courtyard (outdoor) and inside the classroom (indoor) simultaneously in each school pair. Cognitive development was assessed with the n-back and the attentional network tests, in particular, working memory (two-back detectability), superior working memory (three-back detectability), and inattentiveness (hit reaction time standard error). Linear mixed effects models were adjusted for age, sex, maternal education, socioeconomic status, and air pollution exposure at home.Children from highly polluted schools had a smaller growth in cognitive development than children from the paired lowly polluted schools, both in crude and adjusted models (e.g., 7.4% [95% CI 5.6%–8.8%] versus 11.5% [95% CI 8.9%–12.5%] improvement in working memory, p = 0.0024). Cogently, children attending schools with higher levels of EC, NO2, and UFP both indoors and outdoors experienced substantially smaller growth in all the cognitive measurements; for example, a change from the first to the fourth quartile in indoor EC reduced the gain in working memory by 13.0% (95% CI 4.2%–23.1%). Residual confounding for social class could not be discarded completely; however, the associations remained in stratified analyses (e.g., for type of school or high-/low-polluted area) and after additional adjustments (e.g., for commuting, educational quality, or smoking at home), contradicting a potential residual confounding explanation.ConclusionsChildren attending schools with higher traffic-related air pollution had a smaller improvement in cognitive development.

Highlights

  • Air pollution is a suspected developmental neurotoxicant [1]

  • Children attending schools with higher traffic-related air pollution had a smaller improvement in cognitive development

  • On average, working memory increased by 19.0%, superior working memory increased by 15.2%, and inattentiveness decreased by 19.2%

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Air pollution is a suspected developmental neurotoxicant [1]. In animals, inhalation of diesel exhaust and ultrafine particles results in elevated cytokine expression and oxidative stress in the brain [2,3] and altered animal behavior [4,5]. Exposure to traffic-related air pollutants during pregnancy or infancy, when the brain neocortex rapidly develops, has been related to cognitive delays [6,7,8]. The brain regions related to executive functions such as working memory and attention—largely the prefrontal cortex and the striatum [13]—have shown inflammatory responses after traffic-related air pollution exposure [2,14]. We aimed to assess the relationship between long-term exposure to traffic-related air pollutants at school and cognitive development measurements in primary school children within the BREATHE (Brain Development and Air Pollution Ultrafine Particles in School Children) project. We aimed to assess whether exposure of children in primary school to traffic-related air pollutants is associated with impaired cognitive development. The cognitive functions that are essential for learning undergo considerable development between the ages of 6 and 10 years, and further brain changes occur during adolescence

Objectives
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.