Abstract
In Response: We read with interest the letter from Franceschi et al. presenting their findings on alcohol and mortality after breast cancer as a follow-up to our recent article. Our study sought to determine the effect of prediagnostic alcohol consumption, an established breast cancer etiologic risk factor, on the risk of mortality after breast cancer in women diagnosed before age 45 years. In our study, those who consumed alcohol, particularly wine, were at a lower risk of mortality after diagnosis compared with women who did not consume alcohol.One major design difference between our study and that of Franceschi was the targeted age range. The study by Franceschi included women ages 23 to 74 years, whereas ours was restricted to women under age 45 years at breast cancer diagnosis. As we previously noted (1), differences in effects observed for some factors in relation to the development of premenopausal (or younger onset) and postmenopausal (older onset) breast cancer have led most recent epidemiologic studies to pursue age- or menopausal status–specific analyses. In addition, the clear variation in tumor profiles and breast cancer mortality by age such that younger women have more aggressive tumors and substantially higher mortality (2), suggests that it could be similarly important to investigate the effects of prediagnostic factors on mortality stratified by age or menopausal status. The differences in age ranges may have contributed to the differences in our findings.Another key difference between our studies was that the reduced risk of dying we observed in relation to alcohol consumption changed little after adjusting for stage, histologic grade, treatment, and estrogen receptor/progesterone receptor status, whereas Franceschi et al. (3) observed a weakening of any association between alcohol and mortality after adjusting for tumor features. We further assessed our own results by mimicking the model reported by Franceschi et al. (3) and found that adjustment for stage, estrogen receptor/progesterone receptor status, age, and diagnosis year produced little change; the reduced risk of mortality was still observed for women consuming alcohol overall [hazard ratio (HR), 0.6; 95% confidence interval (95% CI), 0.5-0.8] and for wine consumption [HR, 0.7 (95% CI, 0.6-0.9)]. Additionally, in the analysis presented by Franceschi et al. (3) their parsimonious model (adjusting for region, age, and diagnosis year) yielded results in a similar direction to ours, although not as strong in magnitude, for both alcohol consumption overall (HR, 0.88 [95% CI, 0.71-1.08]) and for wine consumption (HR, 0.85 [95% CI, 0.69-1.04]). It is unclear why their associations were attenuated after adjusting for tumor features, whereas ours were not. Perhaps, with their observation of an association between alcohol consumption and estrogen receptor+/progesterone receptor+ tumors (4) and the likelihood that alcohol would be affecting estrogen receptor/progesterone receptor status (not vice versa), their model has adjusted for factors within the causal pathway, which would provide information on the effect of alcohol above and beyond its effects involving the estrogen and/or progesterone receptors.In addition, the difference in consumption levels between our Seattle-based population and the Franceschi study of Italian women are pronounced and may contribute to the differences in effects observed for ever drinkers. Interestingly, in their study, the HRs within women consuming lower levels of alcohol more closely approximated our findings, particularly among women consuming wine [HR of 0.82 (95% CI, 0.63-1.08) for <7 drinks/week; HR of 0.76 (95% CI, 0.58-0.99) for 7-13 drinks/week], as opposed to the HRs they observed in women consuming larger amounts of alcohol.Lastly, the authors comment that wine consumption in their study population is not related to social class, facilitating the assessment of the role of wine consumption in the relative absence of potential confounding due to social class. We were able to investigate the effects of social class, as defined through education and income, on the relationship between wine consumption and mortality but did not find it to alter our prior results. Although wine consumption was associated with income and education level (the proportion of women consuming wine increased with higher income and education level; P < 0.001 for each), the association between wine and reduced mortality persisted after adjustment for income and education in addition to age, diagnosis year, and mammography [HR, 0.7 (95% CI, 0.6-0.9)], as well as in a fuller model with adjustment for income, education, age, diagnosis year, mammography, histology, stage, and treatment [HR, 0.7 (95% CI, 0.5-0.9)]. Thus, we do not feel that the association between wine and reduced mortality observed in our data can be explained away by social class.In closing, this is an understudied topic, and thus, we appreciate Franceschi et al. describing their findings with respect to alcohol and mortality in their breast cancer cohort in an expanded version of their 2008 paper (5).No potential conflicts of interest were disclosed.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.