-I studied the behavior of Purple Martins (Progne subis) at nightfall and the birds' sleeping arrangements, each night, from spring arrival until premigratory flocking began. Martins slept in martin houses until about 15 June, after which they commonly slept in trees. Birds that were firmly established on a territory slept in a room of that territory. Some pairs of birds slept together in the same room and others did not. This behavior was determined by the males. Pairs that occupied their territories for a week or longer often slept together. While building nests, pairs began to prefer certain rooms for sleeping. During egg laying and incubation, all females slept in the nest. They slept with the young and probably brooded them at night until the nestlings were 1315 days old. Some females ceased sleeping with the nestlings after two weeks, and these females either slept in a tree or resumed sleeping with their mates. Many vagrant martins slept in martin houses, often on occupied territories. Martins frequently called from 03:00 until daybreak. I hypothesize that Purple Martins copulate in martin house rooms at night, probably in the early morning. Pairs' sleeping together facilitates mating. Nocturnal copulation benefits males by minimizing cuckoldry and neighbor interference. Females benefit by being less vulnerable to gang rapes. Pairs also may sleep together to conserve heat on cool spring nights. Despite recent studies of Purple Martins (Progne subis), primarily by Finlay (1971, 1976), Niles (1972), Brown (1978a-e, 1979), and E. J. Bitterbaum (pers. comm.), and earlier work by Allen and Nice (1952), Johnston and Hardy (1962) and Johnston (1966), little attention has been paid to sleeping behavior of martins after spring arrival and before premigratory flocking. In fact, sleeping behavior has not been well studied in any North American swallow. A consideration of sleeping patterns in Purple Martins is important for three reasons. First, since martins are thought to copulate within the nesting cavity (Brown 1978a), any sleeping arrangement that uses this chamber could facilitate copulation. Copulation within the cavity may, among other functions, minimize male cuckoldry. Second, sleeping patterns are reliable indicators of an individual's colony residency, and thus are useful in censuses of Purple Martins. Third, additional knowledge of behavior in the genus Progne may be helpful in defining phylogenetic relationships among member species. I present here an investigation of sleeping behavior in Purple Martins. STUDY SITE AND METHODS I made all observations at a Purple Martin colony in Sherman, Grayson Co., north Texas, each year from 1968 to 1976. The colony grew from 2 pairs in 1968, to 3 in 1969-1970, 8 in 1971-1972, 19 in 1973, 20 in 1974, 25 in 1975, and 35 in 1976. A total of 123 pairs (246 individuals) was observed. Although some birds returned year after year, to simplify procedures I treated all birds each year as different individuals; thus the reported total is slightly inflated. All individuals were recognized either by painted aluminum bands, colored plastic leg bands, behavior characteristics, or plumage features. Approximately half of the birds each year were banded or colormarked, and I could also recognize some individuals by their slight habits which became evident after careful observation. These traits included establishing ownership of certain perches on which no other birds were allowed, specific routes to and from the nesting houses, unusually strong aggression toward other martins in the colony and toward other species that came near, and individual patterns of the Claiming-Reclaiming (Johnston and Hardy 1962) display which, for many males early in the season, were uniquely characteristic. Plumage differences were useful in identifying firstyear males because the purple mottling on the head, breast, and belly is variable. Most females were also recognizable by the shading and patterning of gray on the head, breast, and belly. My techniques for capturing adults had a low yield and I could not capture all birds for banding. Only those that were individually recognizable were included in the study. After the martins arrived and became established at the colony, I observed each pair daily and recorded sleeping behavior throughout the season. Toward the end of the season as the colony grew (especially in 1975-1976), it became difficult to observe the behavior of each pair every evening, but all birds were observed at least every other evening. During 1968-76, I noted sleeping behavior on 1,392 nights (24,324 bird-nights). By month, there were 60 nights in February, 262 in March, 253 in April, 242 in May, 270 in June, 265 in July, and 40 in August. I distinguished five major stages during the nesting
Read full abstract