Abstract
The rationale for the present volume is simple: The great majority the everyday reasoning, including that expert groups engaged their professions, is informal. By contrast, most the studies human inference reported by psychologists the literature are formal reasoning. This discrepancy provides considerable cause for concern and not only because cognitive psychology should have some practical application. Excessive focus on formal reasoning tasks has also, our view, inhibited the development good theories human reasoning. What Is Informal Reasoning, and Why Do We Need to Study It? Psychological studies formal reasoning have fallen largely into two domains: deductive reasoning and statistical inference. These two endeavours have much common and some researchers work both areas. In both cases, participants are presented with what problem-solving researchers call well-defined problems. A well-defined problem can be solved by use the information provided and no other; fact, the correct solution to these problems often requires the reasoner to use only the information provided the premises, and to avoid adding background information and knowledge to the problem domain. Instead, a correct solution is achieved by applying a normatively appropriate rule inference. Normative systems are often applied to formal reasoning problems order to define solutions as right or wrong, such that these problems are then construed as tests correct and fallacious reasoning. Hence, these problems are designed to measure the extent to which participants bring to the laboratory an understanding and ability to apply - the relative normative principles. In the case deductive reasoning research, the relevant normative system is formal logic. Participants are given some premises and asked whether a conclusion follows. Under strict deductive reasoning instructions, they are told (a) to assume that the premises are true and (b) to draw or approve only conclusions that necessarily follow. As observed elsewhere (Evans, 2002), this widely used method was developed over 40 years ago when belief logic as a normative and descriptive system for human reasoning was veiy much higher than it is today. In spite the method, much evidence has emerged to support the conclusion that pragmatic factors play a large part human reasoning. We say in spite of because standard deductive instructions aim to suppress precisely those factors that dominate informal reasoning: the introduction prior belief and the expression uncertainty premises and conclusions. In research on statistical inference, a similar story is found. People are asked to make statistical inference on the basis well-defined problems, which relevant probabilities or frequency distributions are provided, and their answers are assessed for correctness against the norms provided by the probability calculus. Research this tradition has been mostly conducted by researchers the heuristics and biases tradition inspired by the work Danny Kahneman and Amos Tversky (Gilovich, Griffin, & Kahneman, 2002; Kahneman, Slovic, & Tversky, 1982). This results an arguably negative research strategy that is similar to much work on deductive reasoning. That is, researchers show primarily what people cannot do (conform to the principles logic or probability theory) and only secondarily address what people actually do. Indeed, one the most common explanations for why intelligent, educated individuals often fail to reason normatively is that they use informal reasoning processes to solve formal reasoning tasks. For example, notwithstanding instructions to the contraiy, reasoners often supplement the information they are provided with background knowledge and beliefs, and make inferences that are consistent with, rather than necessitated by, the premises. If this is the case, it is reasonable to suggest that we study these processes directly, by giving our participants tasks that allow them to express these types behaviours freely, rather than indirectly, via the observation poor performance on a formal task. …
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
More From: Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology / Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.